Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorHutfluss, A
dc.contributor.authorBermúdez‐Cuamatzin, E
dc.contributor.authorMouchet, A
dc.contributor.authorBriffa, Mark
dc.contributor.authorSlabbekoorn, H
dc.contributor.authorDingemanse, NJ
dc.date.accessioned2022-05-09T08:08:18Z
dc.date.available2022-05-09T08:08:18Z
dc.date.issued2022-05-04
dc.identifier.issn0021-8790
dc.identifier.issn1365-2656
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10026.1/19205
dc.description.abstract

<jats:title>Abstract</jats:title><jats:p> <jats:list> <jats:list-item><jats:p>Predictable behaviour (or ‘behavioural stability’) might be favoured in certain ecological contexts, for example when representing a quality signal. Costs associated with producing stable phenotypes imply selection should favour plasticity in stability when beneficial. Repeatable among‐individual differences in degree of stability are simultaneously expected if individuals differ in ability to pay these costs, or in how they resolve cost–benefit trade‐offs.</jats:p></jats:list-item> <jats:list-item><jats:p>Bird song represents a prime example, where stability may be costly yet beneficial when stable singing is a quality signal favoured by sexual selection. Assuming energetic costs, ecological variation (e.g. in food availability) should result in both within‐ and among‐individual variation in stability. If song stability represents a quality signal, we expect directional selection favouring stable singers.</jats:p></jats:list-item> <jats:list-item><jats:p>For a 3‐year period, we monitored 12 nest box plots of great tits <jats:italic>Parus major</jats:italic> during breeding. We recorded male songs during simulated territory intrusions, twice during their mate's laying stage and twice during incubation. Each preceding winter, we manipulated food availability. Assuming that stability is costly, we expected food‐supplemented males to sing more stable songs. We also expected males to sing more stable songs early in the breeding season (when paternity is not decided) and stable singers to have increased reproductive success.</jats:p></jats:list-item> <jats:list-item><jats:p>We found strong support for plasticity in stability for two key song characteristics: minimum frequency and phrase length. Males were plastic because they became more stable over the season, contrary to expectations. Food supplementation did not affect body condition but increased stability in minimum frequency. This treatment effect occurred only in 1 year, implying that food supplementation affected stability only in interaction with (unknown) year‐specific ecological factors.</jats:p></jats:list-item> <jats:list-item><jats:p>We found no support for directional, correlational or fluctuating selection on the stability in minimum frequency (i.e. the song trait whose stability exhibited cross‐year repeatability): stable singers did not have higher reproductive success. Our findings imply that stability in minimum frequency is not a fitness quality indicator unless males enjoy fitness benefits via pathways not studied here. Future studies should thus address the mechanisms shaping and maintaining individual repeatability of song stability in the wild.</jats:p></jats:list-item> </jats:list> </jats:p>

dc.format.extent1507-1520
dc.format.mediumPrint-Electronic
dc.languageen
dc.language.isoen
dc.publisherWiley
dc.subjectanimal communication
dc.subjectanimal personality
dc.subjectbehavioural stability
dc.subjectbird song
dc.subjectdouble hierarchical generalized linear mixed models
dc.subjectnatural selection
dc.subjectreproductive fitness
dc.titleMale song stability shows cross‐year repeatability but does not affect reproductive success in a wild passerine bird
dc.typejournal-article
dc.typeJournal Article
dc.typeResearch Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
plymouth.author-urlhttps://www.webofscience.com/api/gateway?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000799427400001&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=11bb513d99f797142bcfeffcc58ea008
plymouth.issue7
plymouth.volume91
plymouth.publication-statusPublished
plymouth.journalJournal of Animal Ecology
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/1365-2656.13736
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Faculty of Science and Engineering
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Faculty of Science and Engineering/School of Biological and Marine Sciences
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/REF 2021 Researchers by UoA
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/REF 2021 Researchers by UoA/UoA04 Psychology, Psychiatry and Neuroscience
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Research Groups
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Research Groups/Marine Institute
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Users by role
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Users by role/Academics
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Users by role/Researchers in ResearchFish submission
dc.publisher.placeEngland
dcterms.dateAccepted2022-04-18
dc.rights.embargodate2022-5-13
dc.identifier.eissn1365-2656
dc.rights.embargoperiodNot known
rioxxterms.versionofrecord10.1111/1365-2656.13736
rioxxterms.licenseref.urihttp://www.rioxx.net/licenses/all-rights-reserved
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Review


Files in this item

Thumbnail
Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record


All items in PEARL are protected by copyright law.
Author manuscripts deposited to comply with open access mandates are made available in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite only the published version using the details provided on the item record or document. In the absence of an open licence (e.g. Creative Commons), permissions for further reuse of content should be sought from the publisher or author.
Theme by 
Atmire NV