Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorEdworthy, Judy
dc.contributor.authorHellier, Elizabeth
dc.contributor.authorNewbold, L
dc.contributor.authorTitchener, K
dc.date.accessioned2015-02-25T13:10:29Z
dc.date.accessioned2015-02-25T13:11:03Z
dc.date.available2015-02-25T13:10:29Z
dc.date.available2015-02-25T13:11:03Z
dc.date.issued2015-05
dc.identifier.issn0003-6870
dc.identifier.issn1872-9126
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10026.1/3269
dc.description.abstract

Three experiments explore several factors which influence information transmission when warning messages are passed from person to person. In Experiment 1, messages were passed down chains of participants using five different modes of communication. Written communication channels resulted in more accurate message transmission than verbal. In addition, some elements of the message endured further down the chain than others. Experiment 2 largely replicated these effects and also demonstrated that simple repetition of a message eliminated differences between written and spoken communication. In a final field experiment, chains of participants passed information however they wanted to, with the proviso that half of the chains could not use telephones. Here, the lack of ability to use a telephone did not affect accuracy, but did slow down the speed of transmission from the recipient of the message to the last person in the chain. Implications of the findings for crisis and emergency risk communication are discussed.

dc.format.extent252-262
dc.format.mediumPrint-Electronic
dc.languageen
dc.language.isoeng
dc.publisherElsevier BV
dc.relation.replaceshttp://hdl.handle.net/10026.1/3268
dc.relation.replaces10026.1/3268
dc.subjectCommunication channel
dc.subjectWarning design
dc.subjectCrisis and emergency risk communications
dc.titlePassing crisis and emergency risk communications: The effects of communication channel, information type, and repetition
dc.typejournal-article
dc.typeJournal Article
dc.typeResearch Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
plymouth.author-urlhttps://www.webofscience.com/api/gateway?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000350778600027&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=11bb513d99f797142bcfeffcc58ea008
plymouth.volume48
plymouth.publication-statusPublished
plymouth.journalApplied Ergonomics
dc.identifier.doi10.1016/j.apergo.2014.12.009
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Faculty of Health
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/REF 2021 Researchers by UoA
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/REF 2021 Researchers by UoA/UoA04 Psychology, Psychiatry and Neuroscience
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Research Groups
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Research Groups/Centre for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour (CBCB)
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Research Groups/Centre for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour (CBCB)/Behaviour
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Users by role
dc.publisher.placeEngland
dcterms.dateAccepted2014-12-16
dc.identifier.eissn1872-9126
dc.rights.embargoperiodNot known
rioxxterms.versionofrecord10.1016/j.apergo.2014.12.009
rioxxterms.licenseref.urihttp://www.rioxx.net/licenses/all-rights-reserved
rioxxterms.licenseref.startdate2015-05
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Review


Files in this item

Thumbnail
Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record


All items in PEARL are protected by copyright law.
Author manuscripts deposited to comply with open access mandates are made available in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite only the published version using the details provided on the item record or document. In the absence of an open licence (e.g. Creative Commons), permissions for further reuse of content should be sought from the publisher or author.
Theme by 
Atmire NV