Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorChen, Chi-hsin
dc.contributor.authorYu, C
dc.date.accessioned2022-04-11T10:32:38Z
dc.date.available2022-04-11T10:32:38Z
dc.date.issued2022-08
dc.identifier.issn0022-0965
dc.identifier.issn1096-0457
dc.identifier.other105430
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10026.1/19028
dc.description.abstract

Children's everyday learning environment is semantically structured. For example, semantically related things (e.g., fork and spoon) usually co-occur in the same contexts. The current study examines the effects of semantically structured contexts on preschool-age children's (N = 65, 33 girls, age range: 52-68 months) use of statistical information to learn novel word-object mappings. Children were assigned into one of two conditions, in which objects from the same semantic category repeatedly co-occurred in the same trials (Same-category condition) or objects from different categories repeatedly co-occurred in the same trials (Different-categories condition). Children's word learning performance in the two conditions were comparable. However, their errors at test suggested that information extracted by children in the two conditions differed. Importantly, children in the Same-category condition extracted both statistical and semantic relationships from the stimuli.

dc.format.extent105430-105430
dc.format.mediumPrint-Electronic
dc.languageen
dc.language.isoen
dc.publisherElsevier
dc.subjectCross-situational learning
dc.subjectChild word learning
dc.subjectSemantic information
dc.subjectCo-occurrence frequencies
dc.subjectError analyses
dc.subjectcontextual information
dc.titleBuilding lexical networks: Preschoolers extract different types of information in cross-situational learning
dc.typejournal-article
dc.typeJournal Article
dc.typeResearch Support, N.I.H., Extramural
plymouth.author-urlhttps://www.webofscience.com/api/gateway?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000793047600002&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=11bb513d99f797142bcfeffcc58ea008
plymouth.volume220
plymouth.publication-statusPublished
plymouth.journalJournal of Experimental Child Psychology
dc.identifier.doi10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105430
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Faculty of Health
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Faculty of Health/School of Psychology
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Research Groups
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Research Groups/Plymouth Institute of Health and Care Research (PIHR)
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Users by role
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Users by role/Academics
dc.publisher.placeUnited States
dcterms.dateAccepted2022-03-14
dc.rights.embargodate2023-4-11
dc.identifier.eissn1096-0457
dc.rights.embargoperiodNot known
rioxxterms.versionofrecord10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105430
rioxxterms.licenseref.urihttp://www.rioxx.net/licenses/all-rights-reserved
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Review


Files in this item

Thumbnail
Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record


All items in PEARL are protected by copyright law.
Author manuscripts deposited to comply with open access mandates are made available in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite only the published version using the details provided on the item record or document. In the absence of an open licence (e.g. Creative Commons), permissions for further reuse of content should be sought from the publisher or author.
Theme by 
Atmire NV