Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorTsamadi, Den
dc.contributor.authorFalbén, JKen
dc.contributor.authorPersson, LMen
dc.contributor.authorGolubickis, Men
dc.contributor.authorCaughey, Sen
dc.contributor.authorSahin, Ben
dc.contributor.authorMacrae, CNen
dc.date.accessioned2022-02-07T14:21:41Z
dc.date.available2022-02-07T14:21:41Z
dc.date.issued2020-11en
dc.identifier.issn1747-0218en
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10026.1/18657
dc.description.abstract

<jats:p> An extensive literature has demonstrated stereotype-based priming effects. What this work has only recently considered, however, is the extent to which priming is moderated by the adoption of different sequential-priming tasks and the attendant implications for theoretical treatments of person perception. In addition, the processes through which priming arises (i.e., stimulus and/or response biases) remain largely unspecified. Accordingly, here we explored the emergence and origin of stereotype-based priming using both semantic- and response-priming tasks. Corroborating previous research, a stereotype-based priming effect only emerged when a response-priming (vs. semantic-priming) task was used. A further hierarchical drift diffusion model analysis revealed that this effect was underpinned by differences in the evidential requirements of response generation (i.e., a response bias), such that less evidence was needed when generating stereotype-consistent compared with stereotype-inconsistent responses. Crucially, information uptake (i.e., stimulus bias, efficiency of target processing) was faster for stereotype-inconsistent than stereotype-consistent targets. This reveals that stereotype-based priming originated in a response bias rather than the automatic activation of stereotypes. The theoretical implications of these findings are considered. </jats:p>

en
dc.format.extent1939 - 1948en
dc.languageenen
dc.language.isoenen
dc.publisherSAGE Publicationsen
dc.titleStereotype-based priming without stereotype activation: A tale of two priming tasksen
dc.typeJournal Article
plymouth.issue11en
plymouth.volume73en
plymouth.journalQuarterly Journal of Experimental Psychologyen
dc.identifier.doi10.1177/1747021820925396en
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Faculty of Health
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Faculty of Health/School of Psychology
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/REF 2021 Researchers by UoA
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/REF 2021 Researchers by UoA/UoA04 Psychology, Psychiatry and Neuroscience
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/REF 2021 Researchers by UoA/UoA04 Psychology, Psychiatry and Neuroscience/UoA04 Psychology, Psychiatry and Neuroscience MANUAL
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Users by role
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Users by role/Academics
dcterms.dateAccepted2020-04-21en
dc.rights.embargodate2022-02-08en
dc.identifier.eissn1747-0226en
dc.rights.embargoperiodNot knownen
rioxxterms.versionofrecord10.1177/1747021820925396en
rioxxterms.licenseref.urihttp://www.rioxx.net/licenses/all-rights-reserveden
rioxxterms.licenseref.startdate2020-11en
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Reviewen


Files in this item

Thumbnail
Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record


All items in PEARL are protected by copyright law.
Author manuscripts deposited to comply with open access mandates are made available in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite only the published version using the details provided on the item record or document. In the absence of an open licence (e.g. Creative Commons), permissions for further reuse of content should be sought from the publisher or author.
Theme by 
Atmire NV