Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorEdworthy, Judy
dc.contributor.authorBennett, C
dc.contributor.authorDudaryk, R
dc.contributor.authorCrenshaw, N
dc.contributor.authorMcNeer, R
dc.date.accessioned2019-05-02T08:11:39Z
dc.date.issued2019-08
dc.identifier.issn0090-3493
dc.identifier.issn1530-0293
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10026.1/13762
dc.description.abstract

<jats:sec> <jats:title>Objectives:</jats:title> <jats:p>Accurate and timely identification of existing audible medical alarms is not adequate in clinical settings. New alarms that are easily heard, quickly identifiable, and discernable from one another are indicated. The “auditory icons” (brief sounds that serve as metaphors for the events they represent) have been proposed as a replacement to the current international standard. The objective was to identify the best performing icons based on audibility and performance in a simulated clinical environment.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Design:</jats:title> <jats:p>Three sets of icon alarms were designed using empirical methods. Subjects participated in a series of clinical simulation experiments that examined the audibility, identification accuracy, and response time of each of these icon alarms. A statistical model that combined the outcomes was used to rank the alarms in overall efficacy. We constructed the “best” and “worst” performing sets based on this ranking and prospectively validated these sets in a subsequent experiment with a new subject sample.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Setting:</jats:title> <jats:p>Experiments were conducted in simulated ICU settings at the University of Miami.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Subjects:</jats:title> <jats:p>Medical trainees were recruited from a convenience sample of nursing students and anesthesia residents at the institution.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Interventions:</jats:title> <jats:p>In Experiment 1 (formative testing), subjects were exposed to one of the three sets of alarms; identical setting and instruments were used throughout. In Experiment 2 (summative testing), subjects were exposed to one of the two sets of alarms, assembled from the best and worst performing alarms from Experiment 1.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Measurements and Main Results:</jats:title> <jats:p>For each alarm, we determined the minimum sound level to reach audibility threshold in the presence of background clinical noise, identification accuracy (percentage), and response time (seconds). We enrolled 123 medical trainees and professionals for participation (78 with &lt; 6 yr of training). We identified the best performing icon alarms for each category, which matched or exceeded the other candidate alarms in identification accuracy and response time.</jats:p> </jats:sec> <jats:sec> <jats:title>Conclusions:</jats:title> <jats:p>We propose a set of eight auditory icon alarms that were selected through formative testing and validated through summative testing for adoption by relevant regulatory bodies and medical device manufacturers.</jats:p> </jats:sec>

dc.format.extent1050-1057
dc.format.mediumPrint
dc.languageen
dc.language.isoen
dc.publisherLippincott, Williams & Wilkins
dc.subjectalarm fatigue
dc.subjectaudible alarms
dc.subjectauditory alarms
dc.subjectmedical alarms
dc.subjectpatient monitoring
dc.subjectsimulation
dc.titleRecommendation of New Medical Alarms based on Audibility, Identifiability, and Detectability in a Randomized, Simulation-Based Study
dc.typejournal-article
dc.typeJournal Article
dc.typeRandomized Controlled Trial
dc.typeResearch Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
plymouth.author-urlhttps://www.webofscience.com/api/gateway?GWVersion=2&SrcApp=PARTNER_APP&SrcAuth=LinksAMR&KeyUT=WOS:000475675500030&DestLinkType=FullRecord&DestApp=ALL_WOS&UsrCustomerID=11bb513d99f797142bcfeffcc58ea008
plymouth.issue8
plymouth.volume47
plymouth.publication-statusPublished
plymouth.journalCritical Care Medicine
dc.identifier.doi10.1097/CCM.0000000000003802
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Faculty of Health
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Research Groups
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Research Groups/Centre for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour (CBCB)
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Research Groups/Centre for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour (CBCB)/Behaviour
plymouth.organisational-group/Plymouth/Users by role
dc.publisher.placeUnited States
dcterms.dateAccepted2019-04-04
dc.rights.embargodate2020-7-31
dc.identifier.eissn1530-0293
dc.rights.embargoperiodNot known
rioxxterms.versionofrecord10.1097/CCM.0000000000003802
rioxxterms.licenseref.urihttp://www.rioxx.net/licenses/all-rights-reserved
rioxxterms.licenseref.startdate2019-08
rioxxterms.typeJournal Article/Review


Files in this item

Thumbnail
Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record


All items in PEARL are protected by copyright law.
Author manuscripts deposited to comply with open access mandates are made available in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite only the published version using the details provided on the item record or document. In the absence of an open licence (e.g. Creative Commons), permissions for further reuse of content should be sought from the publisher or author.
Theme by 
Atmire NV